Respondent Extinction

Abstract

Respondent extinction refers to the process of removing an antecedent stimulus that is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US) to create a conditioned stimulus (CS) that elicits a conditioned response (CR). This phenomenon is a form of classical conditioning that was originally described by Ivan Pavlov. Unlike operant extinction, which involves the omission or decrease of a reward or punishment, respondent extinction depends on the disassociation of the conditional relationship between the CS and US.

In this article, we will delve into the theory Behind respondent extinction and discuss its implications in various applications, including wildlife conservation, education, and behavioral therapy. Additionally, we will explore the differences between respondent extinction and operant extinction and examine some common methods used in the field of behavior analysis.

Introduction

Respondent extinction is based on the principle that all behaviors are initiated by an unconditioned stimulus. If a neutral stimulus is paired with an US, it will eventually become a conditioned stimulus that elicits a conditioned response. This CR is a learned behavior that is triggered by the presentation of the CS alone, which is not the original US (Thorpe, 1974). Respondent extinction is unique because it does not involve the manipulation of outcomes but rather the disconnection of the CS from the US.

The Theory Behind Respondent Extinction

Pavlov's Classical Conditioning

The concept of respondent extinction is rooted in Ivan Pavlov's classical conditioning research with dogs. Pavlov's most famous experiment involved pairing a neutral stimulus (a tone) with an unconditioned stimulus (meat powder). He discovered that the dogs began to salivate at the sound of the tone, even before the meat powder was presented (Thorpe, 1962). This led Pavlov to propose that dogs established an association between the neutral stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus, creating a conditioned response.

Associative Strength and Spontaneous Recovery

According to Pavlov's theory, the strength of the CR (conditioned response) is determined by the associative strength between the CS (conditioned stimulus) and the US (unconditioned stimulus). Over time, if no US is presented, the associative strength between the CS and US diminishes. However, a single presentation of the US can restore this strength, leading to a recovery of the CR (Thorpe, 1974).

Sensory Preconditioning

Sensory preconditioning is a phenomenon in which a weak stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus for a stronger stimulus. This is often seen in animal studies where it takes several exposures to a less salient CS to elicit a conditioned response to a more salient CS (Lubow, 1973). Preexposure can also lead to habituation, where the responses to a CS decrease over time with repeated exposure.

Applications of Respondent Extinction

Wildlife Conservation

In wildlife conservation, respondent extinction has been used to manage habitats. By removing or reducing cues that predict the availability of resources, such as the sound of a leak, animals have a reduced incentive to disclose their location (Seyfarth & Cheney, 1995). This technique can help protect endangered species by reducing competition and human interference.

Educative purposes

Respondent extinction has been used in educational settings to help children learn to differentiate between real and simulated conditions. For instance, in a classic experiment by Bandura (1969), children experienced<|startoftext|>observational learning and imitated behavior that they observed. By using respondent extinction, researchers could remove a verbal cue that predicted an aversive outcome, reducing the likelihood that the children would imitate the aversive behavior.

Behavioral Therapy

Respondent extinction is a key component in Behavioral Therapy. As part of exposure therapy, patients learn to tolerate and even embrace unwanted behaviors or thoughts, rather than avoiding them (Mendelsohn et al., 2009). By disassociating the CS from the US in behavioral therapy, patients can develop new, more positive behaviors through exposure to the CS alone.

Respondent Extinction vs. Operant Extinction

While respondent extinction and operant extinction share some similarities, they are fundamentally different in their approaches to behavior modification. Operant extinction involves the manipulation of consequences to strengthen or weaken a behavior, whereas respondent extinction depends on the natural disassociation of the CS from the US.

##Respondent extinction is a powerful technique that can be applied in various fields, including wildlife conservation, educational settings, and behavioral therapy. It is based on the principle that behaviors are initiated by unconditioned stimuli and can be modified by removing or reducing cues that predict the occurrence of those stimuli.

References

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1969). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 575-582.

Lubow, E. L. (1973). Sensory preconditioning: Evidence for classical conditioning in the absence of awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9(3), 290-297.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A unified approach to pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 82(2), 204-229.

Mendez, S., Baer, M., & Frost, J. (2001). Response rate as a function of intervals between trials in an extinction procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75(1), 89-94.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes and the Mind. Translated by D. K. Smith. London: Oxford University Press.

Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1995). Social bonds from the bottom up: The role of coalitionary relationships in animal dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(20), 11182-11186.

Thorpe, K. J. (1962). Conditioned responses to neutral and unconditioned stimuli in the white rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 273-280.

Siewerdsen, D., Vervaeke, K., & Verschoor, A. (2013). Nonassociative learning mechanisms. In Handbook of Learning and Intelligence (pp. 213-237). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *